To help offer the best experience possible, Guilford uses cookies on its site. By browsing here, you acknowledge our terms of use. For more information, see our Cookie Policy.
You can also read Guilford's Privacy Policy.

×
Skip to main content

Everything you need with Guilford's 30-day ALL ACCESS pass, just $49.95 | Click on any article to purchase  

Self and Others: Empirical and Neuropsychoanalytic Considerations of Superego and Conscience

Freud's mature theorizing about human morality entrenched the functioning of the superego in anxiety stemming from the fear of punishment, a view with which many later psychoanalysts took issue, producing a debate as to the distinction between superego and conscience. This debate would later be mirrored more broadly in academic psychology concerning distinctions between shame and guilt. This is an area where the clinical observations and theoretical discussions of psychoanalysis have subtly guided research in cognitive psychology and the cognitive and affective neurosciences. These areas, in turn, have both clarified and supported psychoanalytic theory and practice without negating the rich phenomenological and theoretical basis on which psychoanalysis rests.

References

  • Aakvaag H. F., , Thoresen S., , Strøm I. F., , Myhre M., , & Hjemdal O. K. (2019). Shame predicts revictimization in victims of childhood violence: A prospective study of a general Norwegian population sample. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 11(1), 43–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000373 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 Google Scholar
  • Balint M. (1969). The basic fault: Therapeutic aspects of regression. Northwestern University Press. Google Scholar
  • Barnett B. (2007). “You ought to!” A psychoanalytic study of the superego and conscience. Karnac Books. Google Scholar
  • Bluhm R. L., , Williamson P. C., , Osuch E. A., , Frewen P. A., , Stevens T. K., , Boksman K., , Neufeld R. W., , Théberge J., , & Lanius R. A. (2009). Alterations in default network connectivity in posttraumatic stress disorder related to early-life trauma. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 34(3), 187–94. MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Britton R. (1998). Before and after the depressive position: Ps(n)→D(n) →Ps(n+1). In Britton R., Belief and imagination: Explorations in psychoanalysis (pp. 69–81). Routledge. Google Scholar
  • Carveth D. (2013). The still small voice: Psychoanalytic reflections on guilt and conscience. Routledge. Google Scholar
  • Carveth D. (2018). Psychoanalytic thinking: A dialectical critique of contemporary theory and practice. Routledge. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Charles M. (2012). Working with trauma: Lessons from Bion and Lacan. Jason Aronson. Google Scholar
  • Chen C., , Martínez R. M., , & Cheng Y. (2018). The developmental origins of the social brain: Empathy, morality, and justice. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 2584. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02584 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Chen C., , Yang C. Y., , & Cheng Y. (2012). Sensorimotor resonance is an outcome but not a platform to anticipating harm to others. Social Neuroscience, 7, 578–590. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.686924 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Damasio H., , Grabowski T., , Frank R., , Galaburda A., , & Damasio A. (1994). The return of Phineas Gage: Clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science, 264, 1102–1105. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.8178168 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • De Meulemeester C., , Lowyck B., , & Luyten P. (2021). The role of impairments in self-other distinction in borderline personality disorder: A narrative review of recent evidence. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 127, 242–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.022 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Einav S., , & Hood B. M. (2008). Tell-tale eyes: Children's attribution of gaze aversion as a lying cue. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1655–1667. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013299 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Fairbairn W. R. D. (1946). Object-relationships and dynamic structure. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 27, 30–37. MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Frattaroli E. (2013). Reflections on the absence of morality in psychoanalytic theory and practice. In Akhtar S. (Ed.), Guilt: Origins, manifestations, and management (pp. 83–110). Jason Aronson. Google Scholar
  • Frattaroli E. (2015). Do psychoanalysts know what's good for them? If so, why are they always arguing about it? If not, how do they (and we) know what's good for us? In Akhtar S. (Ed.), Human goodness: Origins, manifestations, and clinical implications (pp. 23–52). Rowman & Littlefield. Google Scholar
  • Freud S. (1926). The question of lay analysis: Conversations with an impartial person. In Strachey J. (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, 24 vols. (vol. 20, pp. 177–258). London: Hogarth Press, 1953–1974. Google Scholar
  • Freud S. (1940). An outline of psycho-analysis. Standard ed., 23, 139–207. Google Scholar
  • Goldberg E. (2001). The executive brain: Frontal lobes and the civilized mind. Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  • Howell E. F. (1997). Desperately seeking attachment: A psychoanalytic reframing of the harsh superego. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 10(4), 230–239. Google Scholar
  • Kaplan-Solms K., , & Solms M. (2000). Clinical studies in neuro-psychoanalysis: Introduction to a depth neuropsychology. Karnac Books. Google Scholar
  • Kernberg O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Aronson. Google Scholar
  • Kohut H. (1971). The analysis of the self. International Universities Press. Google Scholar
  • Lei X., , Liao Y., , Zhong M., , Peng W., , Liu Q., , Yao S., , Zhu X., , Tan C., , & Yi J. (2018). Functional connectivity density, local brain spontaneous activity, and their coupling strengths in patients with borderline personality disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, article 342. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00342 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Lewis H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. International Universities Press. Google Scholar
  • Lewis H. B. (1987). On shame and the narcissistic personality. In Nathanson D. (Ed.), The many faces of shame (pp. 93–132). Guilford. Google Scholar
  • Lewis H. B. (1988). The role of shame in symptom formation. In Clynes M., & Panksepp J. (Eds.), Emotions and psychopathology (pp. 95–106). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-1987-1_5 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Panksepp J. (1990). Shame, repression, field dependence, and psychopathology. In Singer J. L. (Ed.), Repression and dissociation: Implications for personality theory, psychopathology, and health (pp. 233–257). University of Chicago Press. Google Scholar
  • Li W., , Mai X., , & Lu C. (2014). The default mode network and social understanding of others: what do brain connectivity studies tell us. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00074 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • McCarthy A., , & Lee K. (2009). Children's knowledge of deceptive gaze cues and its relation to their actual lying behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(2), 117–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.06.005 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Mclatchie N., , Giner-Sorolla R., , & Derbyshire S. W. (2016). ‘Imagined guilt’ vs ‘recollected guilt’: Implications for fMRI. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(5), 703–711. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw001 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • McWilliams N. (2011). Psychoanalytic diagnosis: Understanding personality structure in the clinical process (2nd ed.). Guilford Press. Google Scholar
  • Mesurado B., , & Richaud M. C. (2017). The relationship between parental variables, empathy and prosocial-flow with prosocial behavior toward strangers, friends, and family. Journal of Happiness Studies, 18(3), 843–860. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-016-9748-7 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Michl P., , Meindl T., , Meister F., , Born C., , Engel R. R., , Reiser M., , & Hennig-Fast K. (2014). Neurobiological underpinnings of shame and guilt: A pilot fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(2), 150–157. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss114 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Mills R. S. L. (2003). Possible antecedents and developmental implications of shame in young girls. Infant and Child Development, 12(4), 329–349. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.308 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Mills R. S. L., , Arbeau K. A., , Lall D. I. K., , & De Jaeger A. E. (2010). Parenting and child characteristics in the prediction of shame in early and middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 56(4), 500–528. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2010.0001 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Morey L. C. (2007). Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI): Professional manual (2nd ed.). Psychological Assessment Resources. Google Scholar
  • O'Connor L., , Berry J., , Weiss J., , Bush M., , & Sampson H. (1997). Interpersonal guilt: The development of a new measure. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53, 73–89. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199701)53:13.3.CO;2-O Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Oliveira Silva P., , Maia L., , Coutinho J., , Frank B., , Soares J. M., , Sampaio A., , & Gonçalves Ó. (2018). Empathy by default: Correlates in the brain at rest. Psicothema, 30(1), 97–110. MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Panksepp J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Panksepp J., , & Biven L. (2012). The archeology of mind. Norton. Google Scholar
  • Peters J. R., , & Geiger P. J. (2016). Borderline personality disorder and self-conscious affect: Too much shame but not enough guilt? Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7(3), 303–308. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000176 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Piers G., , & Singer M. (1971). Shame and guilt: A psychoanalytic and a cultural study. Norton. (Original work published 1954) Google Scholar
  • Quesque F., , & Brass M. (2019). The role of the temporoparietal junction in self-other distinction. Brain Topography, 32(6), 943–955. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-019-00737-5 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Reiner A. (2009). The quest for conscience and the birth of the mind. Karnac. Google Scholar
  • Rothrauff T. C., , Cooney T. M., , & An J. S. (2009). Remembered parenting styles and adjustment in middle and late adulthood. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 64(1), 137–146. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbn008 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Sagan E. (1988). Freud, women, and morality: The psychology of good and evil. Basic Books. Google Scholar
  • Schore A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Google Scholar
  • Schore A. N. (2003). Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self. Norton. Google Scholar
  • Schore A. N. (2019). The development of the unconscious mind. Norton. Google Scholar
  • Shabad P. (2020). The forward edge of resistance: Toward the dignity of human agency. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 30(1), 51–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Solms M. (2016). We agree about the metaneuropsychology of the ego (but not so much about the superego): A commentary on “Revisiting the left convexity hypothesis: Changes in the mental apparatus after left dorsomedial prefrontal damage” by Salas & Yuen. Neuropsychoanalysis, 18(2), 133–115. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Solms M. (2018). Depression in neuropsychoanalysis: Why does depression feel bad? In Spagnolo R. (Ed.), Building bridges: The impact of neuropsychoanalysis on psychoanalytic clinical sessions (pp. 39–54). Routledge. Google Scholar
  • Spitzer C., , Jelinek L., , Baumann E., , Benecke C., , & Schmidt A. F. (2021). Negative self-conscious emotions in women with borderline personality disorder as assessed by an Implicit Association Test. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 12(5), 456–465. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000467 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Summers F. (2013). The psychoanalytic vision: The experiencing subject, transcendence, and the therapeutic process. Routledge. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Symington N. (1998). Emotion and spirit: Questioning the claims of psychoanalysis and religion. Routledge. Google Scholar
  • Sznycer D., , Tooby J., , Cosmides L., , Porat R., , & Shalvi S., , & Halperin E. (2016). Shame closely tracks the threat of devaluation by others, even across cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(10), 2625–2630. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1514699113. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Takahashi H., , Yahata N., , Koeda M., , Matsuda T., , Asai K., , & Okubo Y. (2004). Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 23(3), 967–974. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.054 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Tangney J. P., , & Dearing R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. Guilford Press. Google Scholar
  • Tangney J. P., , & Dearing R. L., , Malouf E. T., , Stuewig J., , & Mashek D. (2012). Emotions and morality: You don't have to feel really bad to be good. In Eysenck M. W., , Fajkowska M., , & Maruszewski T. (Eds.), Personality, cognition, and emotion (pp. 141–154). Eliot Werner Publications. Google Scholar
  • Tomkins S. S. (1963). Affect, imagery, consciousness: II. The negative affects. Springer. Google Scholar
  • Tronick E., , Als H., , Adamson L., , Wise S., , & Brazelton T. B. (1978). The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 17, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138(09)62273-1 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Unoka Z., , & Vizin G. (2017). To see in a mirror dimly: The looking glass self is self-shaming in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 258, 322–329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.08.055 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Wagner U., , N'Diaye K., , Ethofer T., , & Vuilleumier P. (2011). Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 21(11), 2461–2470. Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Zellner M. (2022). Freud in the light of neuroscience. In Salas C., , Turnbull O., , & Solms M. (Eds.), Clinical studies in neuropsychoanalysis revisited (pp. 33–59). Routledge. Google Scholar
  • Zhu R., , Feng C., , Zhang S., , Mai X., , & Liu C. (2019). Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analyses. NeuroImage, 186(1), 476–486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.012 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar
  • Zhu R., , Feng C., , Zhang S., , Mai X., , & Liu C., , Wu H., , Xu Z., , Tang H., , Shen X., , Mai X., , & Liu C. (2019). Early distinction between shame and processing in an interpersonal context. Social Neuroscience, 14(1), 53–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2017.1391119 Crossref MedlineGoogle Scholar