A Cooperation Advantage for Theory of Mind in Children and Adults
Abstract
Three studies test whether people engage in mental state reasoning or theory of mind (ToM) differently across two fundamental social contexts: cooperation and competition. Study 1 examines how children with an emerging understanding of false beliefs deploy ToM across these contexts. We find that young preschool children are better able to plant false beliefs in others' minds in a cooperative versus competitive context; this difference does not emerge for other cognitive capacities tested (e.g., executive functioning, memory). Studies 2a and 2b reveal the same systematic difference in adults' ToM for cooperation and competition, even after accounting for relevant predictors (e.g., preference for a task condition, feelings about deception). Together, these findings provide initial evidence for enhanced ToM for cooperation versus competition in early development and also adulthood.
REFERENCES
- 2009). Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 18(5), 175–186. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20222 Google Scholar (
- 2004). Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271(1549), 1693–1699. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2004.2780 Google Scholar (
- Byrne R. W., , & Whiten A. (Eds.). (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press; Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
- 2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053. Google Scholar (
- 2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73–92. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.298 Google Scholar (
- 2004). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(4), 299–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.002 Google Scholar (
- 1998). The role of inhibitory processes in young children's difficulties with deception and false belief. Child Development, 69(3), 672–691. Google Scholar (
- 1989). Small-scale deceit: Deception as a marker of two-, three-, and four-year-olds' early theories of mind. Child Development, 60(6), 1263. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130919 Google Scholar (
- 1994). The role of personal involvement in the assessment of early false belief skills. In Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 403–425). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar (
- 2018). Young children discover how to deceive in 10 days: A microgenetic study. Developmental Science, 21(3), e12566. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12566 Google Scholar (
- 2011). Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: Overestimated effect sizes and the winner's curse. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65(1), 47–55. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1038-5 Google Scholar (
- 1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 312 7-year-olds on a stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X Google Scholar (
- 1998). The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68(1), 70–83. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1997.2423 Google Scholar (
- 2017). Children, chimpanzees, and bonobos adjust the visibility of their actions for cooperators and competitors. Scientific Reports, 7(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-08435-7 Google Scholar (
- 1991). Fledgling theories of mind: Deception as a marker of three-year-olds' understanding of false belief. Child Development, 62(1), 83–97. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01516.x Google Scholar (
- 2014). Agency attribution in infancy: Evidence for a negativity bias. PLoS ONE, 9(5), e96112. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096112 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Google Scholar (
- 1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233–253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00921.x Google Scholar (
- 2016). Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1192–1205. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000140 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2010). Young children have a specific, highly robust bias to trust testimony. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1541–1547. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610383438 Google Scholar (
- 1982). Altruistic behavior and the development of the self in infants. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28(3), 379–388. Google Scholar (
- 1973). Violence without moral restraint: Reflections on the dehumanization of victims and victimizers. Journal of Social Issues, 29(4), 25–61. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1973.tb00102.x Google Scholar (
- 2003). Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language. Analysis, 63, 190–193. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2012). Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: How the rich are different from the poor. Psychological Review, 119(3), 546–572. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028756 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2013). Little liars: Development of verbal deception in children. Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 91–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12023 Google Scholar (
- 2006). Acting intentionally and the side-effect effect: Theory of mind and moral judgment. Psychological Science, 17(5), 421–427. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01722.x Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2000). The emotional side of prejudice: The attribution of secondary emotions to ingroups and outgroups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 186–197. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0402_06 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2006). Ecology, domain specificity, and the origins of theory of mind: Is competition the catalyst? Philosophy Compass, 1(5), 481–492. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-9991.2006.00032.x Google Scholar (
- 2013). When kids know better: Paternalistic helping in 3-year-old children. Developmental Psychology, 49(11), 2071–2081. https://doi.org/]10.1037/a0031715 Google Scholar (
- 2008). Pro-social motive promotes early understanding of false belief. Nature Precedings, 1–16. Google Scholar (
- 2017). The developmental foundations of human fairness. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 0042. Google Scholar (
- 2017). Young children are more likely to spontaneously attribute mental states to members of their own group. Psychological Science, 28(10), 1503–1509. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617710724 Google Scholar (
- 2009). Negativity bias in attribution of external agency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(4), 535–545. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016796 Google Scholar (
- 1990). Moral exclusion and injustice: An introduction. Journal of Social Issues, 46(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb00268.x Google Scholar (
- 1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(04), 515. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512 Google Scholar (
- 2015). Playing by the rules: Self-interest information influences children's trust and trustworthiness in the absence of feedback. Cognition, 134, 140–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.002 Google Scholar (
- 2018). How social status influences our understanding of others' mental states. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 169, 30–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.12.008 Google Scholar (
- 1991). The “windows task” as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(2), 331–349. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1991.tb00881.x Google Scholar (
- 2014). Children's strategic theory of mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(37), 13307–13312. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1403283111 Google Scholar (
- 1989). Intergroup aggression: Its predictors and distinctness from in-group bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(3), 364–373. Google Scholar (
- 1993). Three-year-olds' understanding of mental states: The influence of trickery. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56(2), 135–148. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1993.1029 Google Scholar (
- 2015). Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding? A critical appraisal: Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding. Journal of Zoology, 295(1), 12–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/jzo.12198 Google Scholar (
- 2016). Distinct neural patterns of social cognition for cooperation versus competition. NeuroImage, 137, 86–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.069 Google Scholar (
- 2018). Moral reasoning. In Wixted J. T. (Ed.), Stevens' handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience (pp. 1–25). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119170174.epcn312 Google Scholar (
- 2008). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134(3), 383–403. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.383 Google Scholar (
- 2011). Making oneself predictable: Reduced temporal variability facilitates joint action coordination. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 517–530. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2706-z Google Scholar (
- 2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 1301–1303. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1121448 Google Scholar (
- 2007). Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy, 11(3), 271–294. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x Google Scholar (
- 2009). The roots of human altruism. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 455–471. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712608X379061 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2010). Making sense by making sentient: Effectance motivation increases anthropomorphism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(3), 410–435. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020240 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1979). Intentional communication in the chimpanzee: The development of deception. Cognition, 7(4), 333–362. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(79)90021-0 Google Scholar (
- 2010). Being unpredictable: Friend or foe matters. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1(3), 259–267. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550610370214 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 126–136. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.126 Google Scholar (