When the Going Gets Tough, How Do We Perceive the Future?
Abstract
People struggle to stay motivated to work toward difficult goals. Sometimes the feeling of difficulty signals that the goal is important and worth pursuing; other times, it signals that the goal is impossible and should be abandoned. In this article, we argue that how difficulty is experienced depends on how we perceive and experience the timing of difficult events. We synthesize research from across the social and behavioral sciences and propose a new, integrated model to explain how components of time perception interact with interpretations of experienced difficulty to influence motivation and goal-directed behavior. Although these constructs have been studied separately in previous research, we suggest that these factors are inseparable and that an integrated model will help us to better understand motivation and predict behavior. We conclude with new empirical questions to guide future research and by discussing the implications of this research for both theory and intervention practice.
References
- 2017). No pain no gain? Social demographic correlates and identity consequences of interpreting experienced difficulty as importance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 48, 43–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.08.004 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1993). Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences. New York: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar (
- 2006). Behavioral economics and marketing in aid of decision making among the poor. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25(1), 8–23. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2002). The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13(2), 185–189. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00434 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Google Scholar (
- 2019). Commentary on translational impact: Psychologically wise policies targeting inequality require acknowledgement of scarcity mindsets, mobility beliefs, and class privilege justifications. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 5(3), 289–291. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000202 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2019). A belief in socioeconomic mobility promotes the development of academically motivating identities among low-socioeconomic status youth. Self and Identity, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2019.1664624 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1985). Time perception and attention: The effects of prospective versus retrospective paradigms and task demands on perceived duration. Perception & Psychophysics, 38(2), 115–124. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198848 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579–593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In Moore T. E. (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27–63). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2001). Time flies: Investigating the connection between intrinsic motivation and the experience of time. Journal of Personality, 69(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.00134 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1991). Possible selves across the life span. Human Development, 34(4), 230–255. https://doi.org/10.1159/000277058 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2014). Flow and the foundations of positive psychology. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9088-8 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2008). Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near and far future. Neuro-Image, 40(1), 398–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.025 Google Scholar (
- 2017). To thrive or to suffer at the hand of busyness: How lay theories of busyness influence psychological empowerment and volunteering. ACR North American Advances, NA-45. https://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/1024694/volumes/v45/NA-45 Google Scholar (
- 2009). Don't stop thinking about tomorrow: Individual differences in future self-continuity account for saving. Judgment and Decision Making, 4(4), 280–286. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2001). Scheduling stress: Family and health outcomes of shift work and schedule control. American Behavioral Scientist, 44(7), 1179–1198. https://doi.org/10.1177/00027640121956719 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2017). Social class and identity-based motivation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 61–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.035 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2017). Assessing interpretations of experienced ease and difficulty as motivational constructs. Motivation Science, 3(2), 133–163. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000055 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2006). Age-differential motivational consequences of optimization versus compensation focus in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 21(2), 240–252. https://doi.org/10.1037=/0882-7974.21.2.240 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1990). About time. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar (
- 2017). How to map theory: Reliable methods are fruitless without rigorous theory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(5), 731–741. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2015). Cultural views of life phases. In Smelser N. J., & Baltes p. B. (Eds,), International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (pp. 507–515). New York: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23146-2 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2018). Believing in climate change, but not behaving sustainably: Evidence from a one-year longitudinal study. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 56, 55–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.03.001 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2018). The self over time. Current Opinion in Psychology, 26, 72–75. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2011). Increasing saving behavior through age-progressed renderings of the future self. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, S23–S37. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.48.SPL.S23 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2020). Temporal reframing and participation in a savings program: A field experiment. Marketing Science, 39(6), 1033–1201. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.2019.1177 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2020). Use caution when applying behavioural science to policy. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(11), 1092–1094. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2016). Life stage concepts across history and cultures: Proposal for a new field on indigenous life stages. Human Development, 59(5), 290–316. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2018). Easier seen than done: Merely watching others perform can foster an illusion of skill acquisition. Psychological Science, 29(4), 521–536. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617740646 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2019). When busy is less indulging: Impact of busy mindset on self-control behaviors. Journal of Consumer Research, 45(5), 933–952. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucy069 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2006). The goal-gradient hypothesis resurrected: Purchase acceleration, illusionary goal progress, and customer retention. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(1), 39–58. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.43.1.39 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2017). Conceptual metaphor in social psychology: The poetics of everyday life. New York: Routledge. Google Scholar (
- 2014). The college journey and academic engagement: How metaphor use enhances identity-based motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(5), 679–698. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036414 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2000). Measuring subgroup preferences in conjoint experiments. Political Analysis, 28(2), 207–221. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2018). Seeing more and eating less: Effects of portion size granularity on the perception and regulation of food consumption. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(5), 786–803. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000183 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2015). When does the future begin? Time metrics matter, connecting present and future selves. Psychological Science, 26(6), 816–825. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615572231 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2019). Preparing disadvantaged students for success in college: Lessons learned from the preparation initiative. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(1), 54–59. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618808515 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2006). Public views on climate change: European and USA perspectives. Climatic Change, 77(1–2), 73–95. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-006-9072-z Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2013). The demography of social mobility: Black-White differences in the process of educational reproduction. American Journal of Sociology, 118(6), 1509–1558. https://doi.org/10.1086/670719 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2006). The intergenerational effects of changes in women's educational attainments. American Sociological Review, 71(4), 542–564. https://doi.org/10.1177/000312240607100402 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41(9), 954–969. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2013). Rational temporal predictions can underlie apparent failures to delay gratification. Psychological Review, 120(2), 395–410. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031910 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2006). Globalization, uncertainty and the early life course. In Blossfeld H-P., , Klijzing E., , Mills M., , & Kurz K. (Eds.), Globalization, uncertainty and youth in society: The losers in a globalizing world (pp. 1–23). New York: Routledge. Google Scholar (
- 2018). Rethinking time: Implications for well-being. Consumer Psychology Review, 1(1), 41–53. https://doi.org/10.1002/arcp.1003 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2013). Scarcity: Why having too little means so much. New York: Macmillan. Google Scholar (
- 2007). Is there really a retirement savings crisis? An NRRI analysis (Issue Brief No. 7–11). Center for Retirement Research, Boston College. Google Scholar (
- 2009). The National Retirement Risk Index: After the crash (Issue Brief No. 9–22). Center for Retirement Research, Boston College. Google Scholar (
- 2018). Between the devil and the deep blue sea: Tensions between scientific judgement and statistical model selection. Computational Brain and Behavior, 2(1), 28–34. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2018). From future self to current action: An identity-based motivation perspective. Self and Identity, 17(3), 343–364. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2017.1375003 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2013). Not just any path: Implications of identity-based motivation for disparities in school outcomes. Economics of Education Review, 33, 179–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econedurev.2012.09.002 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2015). Pathways to success through identity-based motivation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2017). Culture three ways: Culture and subcultures within countries. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 435–463. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033617 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2017). Seeing the destination AND the path: Using identity-based motivation to understand and reduce racial disparities in academic achievement. Social Issues and Policy Review, 11(1), 159–194. https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12030 Crossref, Google Scholar , (
- 2017). An identity-based motivation framework for self-regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 28(2–3z), 139–147. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2017.1337406 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2020). Do we report the information that is necessary to give psychology away? A scoping review of the psychological intervention literature 2000-2018. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17, 1–17. Google Scholar (
- 2019). A theory of racialized organizations. American Sociological Review, 84(1), 26–53. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2020). Why hypothesis testers should spend less time testing hypotheses. Perspectives on Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620966795 Google Scholar (
- 2004). Procrastination and intentions to perform health behaviors: The role of self-efficacy and the consideration of future consequences. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(1), 115–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.08.005 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.48.4.813 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2015). Just not worth my time? Experienced difficulty and time investment. Social Cognition, 33(2), 85–103. Link, Google Scholar (
- 2012). Social class disparities in health and education: Reducing inequality by applying a sociocultural self model of behavior. Psychological Review, 119(4), 723–744. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029028 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2016). Not all hours are equal: Could time be a social determinant of health? Sociology of Health & Illness, 38(1), 21–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.12300 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2004). Save More Tomorrow™: Using behavioral economics to increase employee saving. Journal of Political Economy, 112(S1), S164–S187. https://doi.org/10.1086/380085 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440–463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2017). Your money or your time? How both types of scarcity matter to physical activity and healthy eating. Social Science & Medicine, 172, 98–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.10.023 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2014). An approach to test for individual differences in the effects of situations without using moderator variables. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 94–104. Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2016). How being busy can increase motivation and reduce task completion time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(3), 371–384. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000045 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 1993). Relative and absolute duration judgments under prospective and retrospective paradigms. Perception & Psychophysics, 54(5), 656–664. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211789 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2005). Resource slack and propensity to discount delayed investments of time versus money. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(1), 23–37. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.1.23 Crossref, Google Scholar (
- 2003). Gender and elder care in China: The influence of filial piety and structural constraints. Gender & Society, 17(2), 209–229. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243202250734 Crossref, Google Scholar (