To help offer the best experience possible, Guilford uses cookies on its site. By browsing here, you acknowledge our terms of use. For more information, see our Cookie Policy.
You can also read Guilford's Privacy Policy.

×
Skip to main content

ALL ACCESS PASS: All Guilford journals for 30 days, just $49.95 | Click on any article to purchase 

On The Acquisition of Knowledge About Personality Traits: Does Learning About The Self Engage Different Mechanisms Than Learning About Others?

We report the case of R. J., an individual with autism. R. J.'s developmental disorder has impaired his ability to retrieve episodic memories as well as his ability to acquire consensually shared knowledge of animals, foods, and objects (Klein, Cosmides, Costabile, & Mei, 2002). Nevertheless, R. J. has developed normal, consensually accurate knowledge of his own personality traits (Klein, Chan, & Loftus, 1999). Moreover, his self–ratings show that he sees his own personality as distinct from the personalities of others. But R. J.'s facility in learning about his own personality does not translate into a facility in learning about the personality traits of others: He fails to differentiate between the personalities of his various family members, and his ratings of them appear to be less nuanced and less situationally specific than his ratings of his own personality. This pattern is radically at variance from that shown by cognitively normal individuals. Because R. J.'s dissociation is developmental in origin, it can illuminate the nature of the learning mechanisms by which knowledge of personality traits is acquired. It suggests that learning about one's own personality traits may engage a different set of mechanisms than learning about the personality traits of others.

References

  • Ahern, C. A., Wood, F. B., & McBrien, C. M. (1998). Preserved vocabulary and reading acquisition in an amnesic child. In K. Pribram (Ed.), Brain and values (pp. 277–298). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  • Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory and thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  • Babey , S. H., Queller, S., & Klein, S. B. (1998). The role of expectancy violating behaviors in the representation of trait knowledge: A summary-plus-exception model of social memory. Social Cognition, 16, 287–339. LinkGoogle Scholar
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  • Baron-Cohen , S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind?” Cognition, 21, 37–46. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Boucher , J. (1981). Memory for recent events in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 293–301. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Boucher , J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory deficits in early infantile autism: Some similarities to the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 73–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Budesheim , T. L., & Bonnelle, K. (1998). The use of abstract trait knowledge and behavioral exemplars in causal explanations of behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 575–587. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Buss, D. M., & Craik, K. H. (1984). Acts, dispositions, and personality. In B. A. Maher & W. B. Maher (Eds.), Progress in experimental personality research (pp. 241–301). San Diego: Academic Press. Google Scholar
  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Consider the source: The evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective. Vancouver studies in cognitive science (pp. 53–115). New York: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  • Crovitz , H. F., & Shiffman, H. (1974). Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4(5B), 517–518. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Enard , W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S., Lal, C., Wiebe, V., Kitano, T., Monaco, A., & Pbo, S. (2002). Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature418(6900), 869–872; advance online publication 14 August 2002. (doi:10.1038/nature01025). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Gallistel, C. R. (2000). The replacement of general-purpose learning models with adaptively specialized learning modules. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 1179–1191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  • Hirshman , E., & Lanning, K. (1999). Evaluating the role of self-judgments in conscious recollection. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 29–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Johnson , S., & Carey, S. (1998). Knowledge enrichment and conceptual change in folkbiology: Evidence from Williams's syndrome. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 156–200. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Klein, S. B. (2001). A self to remember: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective on how the self creates memory and memory creates self. In C. Sedikides& M. B. Brewer (Eds.), Individual self, relational self, and collective self (pp. 25–46). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Google Scholar
  • Klein, S. B. (in press). The cognitive neuroscience of knowing one's self. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences III. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Babey, S. H., & Sherman, J. W. (1997). The functional independence of trait and behavioral self-knowledge: Methodological considerations and some new findings. Social Cognition, 15, 183–203. LinkGoogle Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Chan, R. L., & Loftus, J. (1999). Independence of episodic and semantic self-knowledge: The case from autism. Social Cognition, 17, 413–436. LinkGoogle Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Cosmides, L., & Costabile, K. A. (2003). Preserved knowledge of self in a case of Alzheimer's dementia. Social Cognition, 21, 151–158. LinkGoogle Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Cosmides, L., Costabile, K. A., & Mei, L. (2002). Is there something special about the self? A neuropsychological case study. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 490–506. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Chance, S. (2001). Priming exceptions: A test of the scope hypothesis in naturalistic trait judgments. Social Cognition, 19, 443–468. LinkGoogle Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Chance, S. (2002). Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychological Review, 109, 306–329. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1993). The mental representation of trait and autobiographical knowledge about the self. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 5, pp. 1–49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Self-knowledge of an amnesic patient: Toward a neuropsychology of personality and social psychology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 250–260. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Loftus, J., Trafton, R. G., & Fuhrman, R. W. (1992). The use of exemplars and abstractions in trait judgments: A model of trait knowledge about the self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 739–753. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Rozendale, K., & Cosmides, L. (2002). A social-cognitive neuroscience analysis of the self. Social Cognition, 20, 105–135. LinkGoogle Scholar
  • Klein , S. B., Sherman, R. W., & Loftus, J. (1996). The role of episodic and semantic memory in the development of trait self-knowledge. Social Cognition, 14, 277–291. LinkGoogle Scholar
  • Leslie , A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “Theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412–426. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Lord, C. G. (1993). The “social self” component of trait knowledge about the self. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), The mental representation of trait and autobiographical knowledge about the self: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 5, pp. 91–100). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  • Marks, I. (1987). Fears, phobias, and rituals. New York: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  • Millward , C., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R. (2000). Recall for self and other in autism: Children's memory for events experienced by themselves and their peers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 15–28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Minshew , N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1993). Is autism an amnesic disorder? Evidence from the California Verbal Learning Test. Neuropsychology, 7, 209–216. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley. Google Scholar
  • McNamara , T. P. (1992). Priming and constraints it places on theories of memory and retrieval. Psychological Review, 99, 650–662. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Ohman , A.& Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious serpent: Snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 5–9. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  • Perner , J., & Ruffman, T. (1994). Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: Developmental evidence and a theory of childhood amnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 516–548. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: Morrow. Google Scholar
  • Schell , T. L., Klein, S. B., & Babey, S. H. (1996). Testing a hierarchical model of self-knowledge. Psychological Science, 7, 170–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  • Tulving , E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 26, 1–12. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Tulving, E. (1993). Self-knowledge of an amnesic individual is represented abstractly. In T. K. Srull& R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 5, pp. 147–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  • Tulving, E. (1995). Organization of memory: Quo vadis? In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 839–847). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  • Tulving , E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Vargha-Khadem , F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K. E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, W., & Mishkin, M. (1997). Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science, 277, 376–380. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
  • Wakabayashi, A. (2003). Is knowledge of personality episodic or semantic? Trait self-knowledge of an amnesic patient—another case in Japan. Unpublished manuscript. Google Scholar
  • Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised (manual). New York: Psychological Corp. Google Scholar
  • Wheeler , M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–354. CrossrefGoogle Scholar